RISS 학술연구정보서비스

검색
다국어 입력

http://chineseinput.net/에서 pinyin(병음)방식으로 중국어를 변환할 수 있습니다.

변환된 중국어를 복사하여 사용하시면 됩니다.

예시)
  • 中文 을 입력하시려면 zhongwen을 입력하시고 space를누르시면됩니다.
  • 北京 을 입력하시려면 beijing을 입력하시고 space를 누르시면 됩니다.
닫기
    인기검색어 순위 펼치기

    RISS 인기검색어

      KCI등재

      Cross-modal Correspondence Between Acoustic Feature and Shape

      한글로보기

      https://www.riss.kr/link?id=A106279543

      • 0

        상세조회
      • 0

        다운로드
      서지정보 열기
      • 내보내기
      • 내책장담기
      • 공유하기
      • 오류접수

      부가정보

      다국어 초록 (Multilingual Abstract)

      Our brain tends to associate stimulus features across the senses in a non-random manner. For example, people show consistency in labelling a rounded shape ‘maluma’/‘bouba’ and a spiky shape ‘takete’/‘kiki’. Previous studies have attrib...

      Our brain tends to associate stimulus features across the senses in a non-random manner. For example, people show consistency in labelling a rounded shape ‘maluma’/‘bouba’ and a spiky shape ‘takete’/‘kiki’. Previous studies have attributed this phenomenon to the correspondence between sound and shape, but without controlling for other potential factors (i.e., linguistic/orthographical factors). The present study examines the role of acoustic aspect per se by manipulating articulatory gestures to generate synthetic speech sounds not confined to a specific language. Participants were asked to choose either a rounded or spiky shape to indicate the shape that better matched each synthetic speech sound. The results demonstrate that shape choice was systematically mapped on to the dimensions manipulated to generate the sounds. These results indicate that acoustic features indeed drive the association between sound and visual shape.

      더보기

      참고문헌 (Reference)

      1 Styles, S. J., "When does Maluma/Takete fail? Two key failures and a meta-analysis suggest that phonology and phonotactics matter" 8 : 1-17, 2017

      2 Lee Masson, H., "Visual and haptic shape processing in the human brain : Unisensory processing, multisensory convergence, and top-down influences" 26 : 3402-3412, 2016

      3 Gaißert, N., "Visual and haptic perceptual spaces show high similarity in humans" 10 : 1-20, 2010

      4 Nielsen, A., "The sound of round : Evaluating the sound-symbolic role of consonants in the classic Takete-Maluma phenomenon" 65 : 115-124, 2011

      5 Maurer, D., "The shape of boubas : Sound-shape correspondences in toddlers and adults" 9 : 316-322, 2006

      6 Brainard, D. H., "The psychophysics toolbox" 10 : 433-436, 1997

      7 Davis, R., "The fitness of names to drawings. A cross-cultural study in Tanganyika" 52 : 259-268, 1961

      8 Taitz, A., "The audiovisual structure of onomatopoeias: An intrusion of real-world physics in lexical creation" 13 : e0193466-, 2018

      9 Pelli, D. G., "The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics : Transforming numbers into movies" 10 : 437-442, 1997

      10 Nam, H., "TADA : An enhanced, portable Task Dynamics model in MATLAB" 115 : 2430-2430, 2004

      1 Styles, S. J., "When does Maluma/Takete fail? Two key failures and a meta-analysis suggest that phonology and phonotactics matter" 8 : 1-17, 2017

      2 Lee Masson, H., "Visual and haptic shape processing in the human brain : Unisensory processing, multisensory convergence, and top-down influences" 26 : 3402-3412, 2016

      3 Gaißert, N., "Visual and haptic perceptual spaces show high similarity in humans" 10 : 1-20, 2010

      4 Nielsen, A., "The sound of round : Evaluating the sound-symbolic role of consonants in the classic Takete-Maluma phenomenon" 65 : 115-124, 2011

      5 Maurer, D., "The shape of boubas : Sound-shape correspondences in toddlers and adults" 9 : 316-322, 2006

      6 Brainard, D. H., "The psychophysics toolbox" 10 : 433-436, 1997

      7 Davis, R., "The fitness of names to drawings. A cross-cultural study in Tanganyika" 52 : 259-268, 1961

      8 Taitz, A., "The audiovisual structure of onomatopoeias: An intrusion of real-world physics in lexical creation" 13 : e0193466-, 2018

      9 Pelli, D. G., "The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics : Transforming numbers into movies" 10 : 437-442, 1997

      10 Nam, H., "TADA : An enhanced, portable Task Dynamics model in MATLAB" 115 : 2430-2430, 2004

      11 Ramachandran, V. S., "Synaesthesia-a window into perception, thought and language" 8 : 3-34, 2001

      12 Ozturk, O., "Sound symbolism in infancy : Evidence for sound-shape cross-modal correspondences in 4-month-olds" 114 : 173-186, 2013

      13 Hinton, L., "Sound Symbolism" Cambridge University Press 1994

      14 Cuskley, C., "Phonological and orthographic influences in the bouba-kiki effect" 81 : 119-130, 2017

      15 Tarte, R., "Phonetic symbolism in adult native speakers of Czech" 17 : 87-94, 1974

      16 D’Onofrio, A., "Phonetic detail and dimensionality in sound-shape correspondences : Refining the Bouba-Kiki paradigm" 57 : 367-393, 2014

      17 Nielsen, A., "Parsing the role of consonants versus vowels in the classic Takete-Maluma phenomenon" 67 : 153-163, 2013

      18 Ashby, F. G., "On the dangers of averaging across subjects when using multidimensional scaling or the similarity-choice model" 5 : 144-151, 1994

      19 Cox, T. F., "Multidimensional scaling" Chapman & Hall 2001

      20 Fort, M., "Looking for the bouba-kiki effect in pre-lexical infants" 71-76, 2013

      21 Köhler, W., "Gestalt psychology" Liveright 1947

      22 Köhler, W., "Gestalt psychology" Liveright 1929

      23 Donaldson, G. S., "Effects of vowel context on the recognition of initial and medial consonants by cochlear implant users" 27 : 658-677, 2006

      24 Spence, C., "Crossmodal correspondences: A tutorial review" 73 : 971-995, 2011

      25 Ahlner, F., "Cross-modal iconicity: A cognitive semiotic approach to sound symbolism" 38 (38): 298-348, 2010

      26 Fort, M., "Consonants are more important than vowels in the Bouba-kiki Effect" 58 : 247-266, 2015

      27 Nam, H., "Computational simulation of CV combination preferences in babbling" 41 : 63-77, 2013

      28 Bremner, A. J., ""Bouba"and"Kiki"in Namibia? A remote culture make similar shape-sound matches, but different shape-taste matches to Westerners" 126 : 165-172, 2013

      더보기

      동일학술지(권/호) 다른 논문

      분석정보

      View

      상세정보조회

      0

      Usage

      원문다운로드

      0

      대출신청

      0

      복사신청

      0

      EDDS신청

      0

      동일 주제 내 활용도 TOP

      더보기

      주제

      연도별 연구동향

      연도별 활용동향

      연관논문

      연구자 네트워크맵

      공동연구자 (7)

      유사연구자 (20) 활용도상위20명

      인용정보 인용지수 설명보기

      학술지 이력

      학술지 이력
      연월일 이력구분 이력상세 등재구분
      2026 평가예정 재인증평가 신청대상 (재인증)
      2020-01-01 평가 등재학술지 유지 (재인증) KCI등재
      2017-01-01 평가 등재학술지 유지 (계속평가) KCI등재
      2013-01-01 평가 등재학술지 유지 (등재유지) KCI등재
      2010-01-01 평가 등재학술지 유지 (등재유지) KCI등재
      2009-04-02 학술지명변경 한글명 : 한국심리학회지:실험 -> 한국심리학회지: 인지 및 생물
      외국어명 : Korean Journal of Experimental Psychology -> Korean Journal of Cognitive and Biological Psychology
      KCI등재
      2009-02-02 학회명변경 한글명 : 한국실험심리학회 -> 한국인지및생물심리학회
      영문명 : Korean Society For Experimental Psychology -> Korean Society For Cognitive and Biological Psychology
      KCI등재
      2008-01-01 평가 등재학술지 유지 (등재유지) KCI등재
      2006-01-01 평가 등재학술지 유지 (등재유지) KCI등재
      2004-01-01 평가 등재학술지 유지 (등재유지) KCI등재
      2001-07-01 평가 등재학술지 선정 (등재후보2차) KCI등재
      1999-01-01 평가 등재후보학술지 선정 (신규평가) KCI등재후보
      더보기

      학술지 인용정보

      학술지 인용정보
      기준연도 WOS-KCI 통합IF(2년) KCIF(2년) KCIF(3년)
      2016 0.44 0.44 0.39
      KCIF(4년) KCIF(5년) 중심성지수(3년) 즉시성지수
      0.36 0.44 0.731 0.11
      더보기

      이 자료와 함께 이용한 RISS 자료

      나만을 위한 추천자료

      해외이동버튼